This Event is licensed under the Creative Commons BY-SA license. This license allows reusers to distribute, remix, adapt, and build upon the material in any medium or format, so long as attribution is given to the creator. The license allows for commercial use. If you remix, adapt, or build upon the material, you must license the modified material under identical terms.
Event: 402
Key Event Title
Cognitive function, decreased
Short name
Biological Context
Level of Biological Organization |
---|
Individual |
Key Event Components
Process | Object | Action |
---|---|---|
learning or memory | decreased | |
cognition | decreased |
Key Event Overview
AOPs Including This Key Event
AOP Name | Role of event in AOP | Point of Contact | Author Status | OECD Status |
---|---|---|---|---|
TPO Inhibition and Altered Neurodevelopment | AdverseOutcome | Kevin Crofton (send email) | Open for citation & comment | WPHA/WNT Endorsed |
NIS and Neurodevelopment | AdverseOutcome | Kevin Crofton (send email) | Not under active development | |
NIS and Cognitive Dysfunction | AdverseOutcome | Mary Gilbert (send email) | Under Development: Contributions and Comments Welcome | |
Transthyretin interference | AdverseOutcome | Kristie Sullivan (send email) | Under Development: Contributions and Comments Welcome | Under Development |
TR Antagonism and DNT | AdverseOutcome | Kevin Crofton (send email) | Under development: Not open for comment. Do not cite | Under Development |
Organo-Phosphate Chemicals leading to impaired cognitive function | AdverseOutcome | SAROJ AMAR (send email) | Under development: Not open for comment. Do not cite | |
TR antagonism leading to decreased cognition | AdverseOutcome | Eliska Kuchovska (send email) | Under development: Not open for comment. Do not cite | |
Binding to laminin and DNT | AdverseOutcome | Eliska Kuchovska (send email) | Under development: Not open for comment. Do not cite | |
Cholesterol metabolism and DNT | AdverseOutcome | Eliska Kuchovska (send email) | Under Development: Contributions and Comments Welcome | |
Increased ROS and DNT | AdverseOutcome | Eliska Kuchovska (send email) | Under development: Not open for comment. Do not cite | |
Voltage-gated sodium channels and DNT | AdverseOutcome | Eliska Kuchovska (send email) | Under development: Not open for comment. Do not cite | |
AhR activation in the liver leading to Adverse Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Mammals | AdverseOutcome | Prakash Patel (send email) | Under development: Not open for comment. Do not cite | |
AhR activation in the thyroid leading to Adverse Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Mammals | AdverseOutcome | Prakash Patel (send email) | Under development: Not open for comment. Do not cite | |
Binding to voltage gate sodium channels during development leads to cognitive impairment | AdverseOutcome | Iris Mangas (send email) | Under development: Not open for comment. Do not cite | Under Review |
Taxonomic Applicability
Life Stages
Life stage | Evidence |
---|---|
All life stages | High |
Sex Applicability
Term | Evidence |
---|---|
Male | High |
Female | High |
Key Event Description
Learning and memory depend upon the coordinated action of different brain regions and neurotransmitter systems constituting functionally integrated neural networks (D’Hooge and DeDeyn, 2001). Among the many brain areas engaged in the acquisition or retrieval of a learned event, the hippocampal-based memory systems have received the most study. The main learning areas and pathways are similar in rodents and primates, including man (Eichenbaum, 2000; Stanton and Spear, 1990; Squire, 2004; Gilbert., 2006).
In humans, the hippocampus is involved in recollection of an event’s rich spatial-temporal contexts and distinguished from simple semantic memory which is memory of a list of facts (Burgess et al., 2000). Hemispheric specialization has occurred in humans, with the left hippocampus specializing in verbal and narrative memories (i.e., context-dependent episodic or autobiographical memory) and the right hippocampus, more prominently engaged in visuo-spatial memory (i.e., memory for locations within an environment). The hippocampus is particularly critical for the formation of episodic memory, and autobiographical memory tasks have been developed to specifically probe these functions (Eichenbaun, 2000; Willoughby et al., 2014). In rodents, there is obviously no verbal component in hippocampal memory, but reliance on the hippocampus for spatial, temporal and contextual memory function has been well documented. Spatial memory deficits and fear-based context learning paradigms engage the hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex (Eichenbaum, 2000; Shors et al., 2001; Samuels et al., 2011; Vorhees and Williams, 2014; D’Hooge and DeDeyn, 2001; Lynch, 2004; O’Keefe and Nadal, 1978). These tasks are impaired in animals with hippocampal dysfunction (O’Keefe and Nadal, 1978; Morris and Frey, 1987; Gilbert et al., 2016).
How It Is Measured or Detected
In rodents, a variety of tests of learning and memory have been used to probe the integrity of hippocampal function. These include tests of spatial learning like the radial arm maze (RAM), the Barnes maze, and most commonly, the Morris water maze (MWM). Tests such as novel object recognition, and fear-based context learning are also sensitive to hippocampal disruption. Finally, trace fear conditioning which incorporates a temporal component upon traditional amygdala-based fear learning engages the hippocampus. The text below provides brief descriptions of the most used tasks.
- RAM, Barnes Maze, and MWM are examples of spatial tasks in which animals are required to learn: the location of a food reward (RAM); an escape hole to enter a preferred dark tunnel from a brightly lit open field area (Barnes maze); or a hidden platform submerged below the surface of the water in a large tank of water (MWM) (Vorhees and Williams, 2014).
- Novel Object Recognition (NOR) and its variants are widely used in neuroscience, although their suitability for safety assessment remains unclear (Vorhees and Williams, 2024). NOR and novel place recognition (NPR) are examples of ‘incidental learning’ and rely on the dorsal hippocampus. They are simple tasks and are used to probe recognition memory. Two objects are presented to animals in an open field on trial 1, and animals are allowed time to briefly explore them. On trial 2, one object is replaced with a novel object and time spent interacting with the novel object is taken evidence of memory retention (i.e., one of these objects is familiar, the other is novel (Cohen and Stackman, 2015). In novel place recognition, the objects are shifted to a location within the arena. Compared to tests of spatial learning, the learning event is transient, the results often variable, and the test has a very narrow dynamic range.
- Contextual Fear Conditioning is a hippocampal based learning task in which animals are placed in a novel environment and allowed to explore for several minutes before delivery of an aversive stimulus, typically a mild foot shock. Upon reintroduction to this same environment in the future (typically 24-48 hours after original training), animals will limit their exploration, the context of this chamber being associated with an aversive event (unconditional stimulus, US). The degree of suppression of activity after training is taken as evidence of retention, i.e., memory (Curzon et al., 2009).
- Trace Fear Conditioning. Standard fear conditioning paradigms require animals to make an association between a neutral conditioning stimulus (CS, e.g., a light or a tone) and an aversive stimulus (US, e.g., a foot shock). The unconditioned response (CRUR) that is elicited upon delivery of the foot shock US is freezing behavior. With repetition of CS/US delivery, the previously neutral stimulus comes to elicit the freezing response. This type of learning is dependent on the amygdala, a brain region associated with, but distinct from the hippocampus. Introducing a brief delay between presentation of the neutral CS and the aversive US, a trace period, requires the engagement of the amygdala and the hippocampus (Shors et al., 2004).
Most methods used in animals are well established in the published literature, and many have been engaged to evaluate the effects of developmental neurotoxicants. The US EPA and OECD Developmental Neurotoxicity (DNT) Guidelines (OCSPP 870.6300 or OECD 426) both require testing of learning and memory (USEPA, 1998; OECD, 2007). These DNT Guidelines have been deemed valid to identify DNT and adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes (Makris et al., 2009).
A variety of standardized learning and memory tests have been developed for human neuropsychological testing. These include episodic autobiographical memory, word pair recognition memory; object location recognition memory. Some components of these tests have been incorporated in general tests of adult intelligence (IQ) such as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) which calculates four composite scores that examine various domains within an individual’s overall cognitive ability: Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI), Working Memory Index (WMI), and Processing Speed Index (PSI) (Climie and Rostad, 2011). Modifications have been made and norms developed for incorporating tests of learning and memory in children. Examples of some of these tests include:
- Rey Osterieth Complex Figure (RCFT) which probes a variety of functions including visuospatial abilities, memory, attention, planning, and working memory (Shin et al., 2006).
- Children’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test (CAVLT) is a free recall of presented word lists that yields measures of Immediate Memory Span, Level of Learning, Immediate Recall, Delayed Recall, Recognition Accuracy, and Total Intrusions. (Lezak 1994; Talley, 1986).
- Continuous Visual Memory Test (CVMT) measures visual learning and memory. It is a free recall of presented pictures/objects rather than words but that yields similar measures of Immediate Memory Span, Level of Learning, Immediate Recall, Delayed Recall, Recognition Accuracy, and Total Intrusions. (Lezak, 1984; 1994).
- Story Recall from Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) Logical Memory Test Battery, a standardized neuropsychological test designed to measure memory functions (Lezak, 1994; Talley, 1986).
- Autobiographical memory (AM) is the recollection of specific personal events in a multifaceted higher order cognitive process. It includes episodic memory- remembering of past events specific in time and place, in contrast to semantic autobiographical memory is the recollection of personal facts, traits, and general knowledge. Episodic AM is associated with greater activation of the hippocampus and a later and more gradual developmental trajectory. Absence of episodic memory in early life (infantile amnesia) is thought to reflect immature hippocampal function (Herold et al., 2015; Fivush, 2011).
- Staged AM Task. In this version of the AM test, children participate in a staged event involving a tour of the hospital, perform a series of tasks (counting footprints in the hall, identifying objects in wall display, buying lunch, watched a video). It is designed to contain unique event happenings, place, time, visual/sensory/perceptual details. Four to five months later, interviews are conducted using Children’s Autobiographical Interview and scored according to standardized scheme (Willoughby et al., 2014).
Domain of Applicability
Basic forms of learning behavior such as habituation have been found in many taxa from worms to humans (Alexander, 1990). More complex cognitive processes such as executive function likely reside only in higher mammalian species such as non-human primates and humans.Basic forms of learning behavior such as habituation have been found in many taxa from worms to humans (Alexander, 1990). More complex cognitive processes such as executive function likely reside only in higher mammalian species such as non-human primates and humans.
Regulatory Significance of the Adverse Outcome
A prime example of impairments in cognitive function as the adverse outcome for regulatory action is developmental lead exposure and IQ function in children (Bellinger, 2012). In addition, testing for the impact of chemical exposures on cognitive function, often including spatially-mediated behaviors, is an integral part of both EPA and OECD developmental neurotoxicity guidelines (USEPA, 1998; OECD, 2007).
References
Alexander RD (1990) Epigenetic rules and Darwinian algorithms: The adaptive study of learning and development. Ethology and Sociobiology 11:241-303.
Bellinger DC (2012) A strategy for comparing the contributions of environmental chemicals and other risk factors to neurodevelopment of children. Environ Health Perspect 120:501-507.
Burgess N (2002) The hippocampus, space, and viewpoints in episodic memory. Q J Exp Psychol A 55:1057-1080.
Climie, E. A., & Rostad, K. (2011). Test Review: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 29(6), 581–586. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282911408707
Cohen, SJ and Stackman, RW. (2015). Assessing rodent hippocampal involvement in the novel object recognition task. A review. Behav. Brain Res. 285: 105-1176.
Curzon P, Rustay NR, Browman KE. Cued and Contextual Fear Conditioning for Rodents. In: Buccafusco JJ, editor. Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience. 2nd edition. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2009
D'Hooge R, De Deyn PP (2001) Applications of the Morris water maze in the study of learning and memory. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 36:60-90.
Eichenbaum H (2000) A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:41-50.
Fivush R. The development of autobiographical memory. Annu Rev Psychol. 2011. 62:559-82.
Gilbert ME, Sanchez-Huerta K, Wood C (2016) Mild Thyroid Hormone Insufficiency During Development Compromises Activity-Dependent Neuroplasticity in the Hippocampus of Adult Male Rats. Endocrinology 157:774-787.
Gilbert ME, Sui L (2006) Dose-dependent reductions in spatial learning and synaptic function in the dentate gyrus of adult rats following developmental thyroid hormone insufficiency. Brain Res 1069:10-22.
Herold, C, Lässer, MM, Schmid, LA, Seidl, U, Kong, L, Fellhauer, I, Thomann, PA, Essig, M and Schröder, J. (2015). Neuropsychology, Autobiographical Memory, and Hippocampal Volume in “Younger” and “Older” Patients with Chronic Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry, 6: 53.
Lezak MD (1984) Neuropsychological assessment in behavioral toxicology--developing techniques and interpretative issues. Scand J Work Environ Health 10 Suppl 1:25-29.
Lezak MD (1994) Domains of behavior from a neuropsychological perspective: the whole story. Nebr Symp Motiv 41:23-55.
Lynch, M.A. (2004). Long-Term Potentiation and Memory. Physiological Reviews. 84:87-136.
Makris SL, Raffaele K, Allen S, Bowers WJ, Hass U, Alleva E, Calamandrei G, Sheets L, Amcoff P, Delrue N, Crofton KM. A retrospective performance assessment of the developmental neurotoxicity study in support of OECD test guideline 426. Environ Health Perspect. 2009 Jan;117(1):17-25.
Morris RG, Frey U. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity: role in spatial learning or the automaticrecording of attended experience? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1997 Oct 29;352(1360):1489-503. Review
O’Keefe, J. and Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
OECD. 2007. OECD guidelines for the testing of chemicals/ section 4: Health effects. Test no. 426: Developmental neurotoxicity study. www.Oecd.Org/dataoecd/20/52/37622194.Pdf [accessed May 21, 2012].
OECD Initial Recommendations on Evaluation of Data from the Developmental Neurotoxicity (DNT) In-Vitro Testing Battery; Series on Testing and Assessment No. 377. 2023. Available at: https://one.oecd.org/document/ENV/CBC/MONO(2023)13/en/pdf
Samuels BA, Hen R (2011) Neurogenesis and affective disorders. Eur J Neurosci 33:1152-1159.
Shin, MS, Park, SY, Park, SR, Oeol, SH and Kwon, JS. (2006). Clinical and empirical appliations fo the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test. Nature Protocols, 1: 892-899.
Shors TJ, Miesegaes G, Beylin A, Zhao M, Rydel T, Gould E (2001) Neurogenesis in the adult is involved in the formation of trace memories. Nature 410:372-376.
Squire LR (2004) Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. Neurobiol Learn Mem 82:171-177.
Stanton ME, Spear LP (1990) Workshop on the qualitative and quantitative comparability of human and animal developmental neurotoxicity, Work Group I report: comparability of measures of developmental neurotoxicity in humans and laboratory animals. Neurotoxicol Teratol 12:261-267.
Talley, JL. (1986). Memory in learning disabled children: Digit span and eh Rey Auditory verbal learning test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Elseiver.
U.S.EPA. 1998. Health effects guidelines OPPTS 870.6300 developmental neurotoxicity study. EPA Document 712-C-98-239.Office of Prevention Pesticides and Toxic Substances.
Vorhees CV, Williams MT (2014) Assessing spatial learning and memory in rodents. ILAR J 55:310-332.
Vorhees, C and Williams M. Tests for Learning and Memory in Rodent Regulatory Studies. Current Research in Toxicology, 2024, Curr Res Toxicol. 2024; 6: 100151. doi: 10.1016/j.crtox.2024.100151
Willoughby KA, McAndrews MP, Rovet JF. Accuracy of episodic autobiographical memory in children with early thyroid hormone deficiency using a staged event. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2014. 9:1-11.